1.14: Causa y Efecto

Cause and Effect
JoVE Core
Social Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Core Social Psychology
Cause and Effect

10,897 Views

01:53 min
February 12, 2020

Overview

While variables are sometimes correlated because one does cause the other, it could also be that some other factor, a confounding variable, is actually causing the systematic movement in our variables of interest. For instance, as sales in ice cream increase, so does the overall rate of crime. Is it possible that indulging in your favorite flavor of ice cream could send you on a crime spree? Or, after committing crime do you think you might decide to treat yourself to a cone?

There is no question that a relationship exists between ice cream and crime (e.g., Harper, 2013), but it would be pretty foolish to decide that one thing actually caused the other to occur. It is much more likely that both ice cream sales and crime rates are related to the temperature outside. In this case, temperature is a confounding variable that could account for the relationship between the two variables. When the temperature is warm, there are lots of people out of their houses, interacting with each other, getting annoyed with one another, and sometimes committing crimes. Also, when it is warm outside, we are more likely to seek a cool treat like ice cream. How do we determine if there is indeed a relationship between two things? And when there is a relationship, how can we discern whether it is attributable to coincidence or causation?

Even when we cannot point to clear confounding variables, we should not assume that a correlation between two variables implies that one variable causes changes in another. This can be frustrating when a cause-and-effect relationship seems clear and intuitive. Think about the American Cancer Society and how their research projects were some of the first demonstrations of the link between smoking and cancer. It seems reasonable to assume that smoking causes cancer, but if we were limited to correlational research, we would be overstepping our bounds by making this assumption.

Unfortunately, people mistakenly make claims of causation as a function of correlations all the time. Such claims are especially common in advertisements and news stories. For example, recent research found that people who eat cereal on a regular basis achieve healthier weights than those who rarely eat cereal (Frantzen, Treviño, Echon, Garcia-Dominic, & DiMarco, 2013; Barton et al., 2005). Guess how the cereal companies report this finding. Does eating cereal really cause an individual to maintain a healthy weight, or are there other possible explanations, such as, someone at a healthy weight is more likely to regularly eat a healthy breakfast than someone who is obese or someone who avoids meals in an attempt to diet? While correlational research is invaluable in identifying relationships among variables, a major limitation is the inability to establish causality. Psychologists want to make statements about cause and effect, but the only way to do that is to conduct an experiment to answer a research question. Scientific experiments incorporate methods that eliminate, or control for, alternative explanations, which allow researchers to explore how changes in one variable cause changes in another variable.

Illusory Correlations

The temptation to make erroneous cause-and-effect statements based on correlational research is not the only way we tend to misinterpret data. We also tend to make the mistake of illusory correlations, especially with unsystematic observations. Illusory correlations, or false correlations, occur when people believe that relationships exist between two things when no such relationship exists. One well-known illusory correlation is the supposed effect that the moon’s phases have on human behavior. Many people passionately assert that human behavior is affected by the phase of the moon, and specifically, that people act strangely when the moon is full.

There is no denying that the moon exerts a powerful influence on our planet. The ebb and flow of the ocean’s tides are tightly tied to the gravitational forces of the moon. Many people believe, therefore, that it is logical that we are affected by the moon as well. After all, our bodies are largely made up of water. A meta-analysis of nearly 40 studies consistently demonstrated, however, that the relationship between the moon and our behavior does not exist (Rotton & Kelly, 1985). While we may pay more attention to odd behavior during the full phase of the moon, the rates of odd behavior remain constant throughout the lunar cycle.

Why are we so apt to believe in illusory correlations like this? Often we read or hear about them and simply accept the information as valid. Or, we have a hunch about how something works and then look for evidence to support that hunch, ignoring evidence that would tell us our hunch is false; this is known as confirmation bias. Other times, we find illusory correlations based on the information that comes most easily to mind, even if that information is severely limited. And while we may feel confident that we can use these relationships to better understand and predict the world around us, illusory correlations can have significant drawbacks. For example, research suggests that illusory correlations—in which certain behaviors are inaccurately attributed to certain groups—are involved in the formation of prejudicial attitudes that can ultimately lead to discriminatory behavior (Fiedler, 2004).

 

This text is adapted from OpenStax, Psychology. OpenStax CNX.

Transcript

En algunos casos, la relación entre dos elementos parece clara e intuitiva, como cuando hay un juego de deportes, la gente pide pizza aunque se esté haciendo tarde. Un miembro del grupo jura que comer varias rebanadas antes de retirarse a dormir le da pesadillas.

Ahora, la única forma de responder a esta pregunta “¿Comer pizza justo antes de ir a dormir hace que alguien tenga más pesadillas?” es diseñar un experimento.

En un tipo de diseño experimental, una relación de causa y efecto, un investigador puede determinar si la manipulación de una variable independiente (en este caso, comer pizza antes de acostarse) causa un efecto particular: cambios en la variable dependiente, el número de pesadillas que ocurren a lo largo de la noche.

Podrían asignar la mitad de los participantes al grupo experimental, al que se le asigna la manipulación experimental (la tarea de comer tres rebanadas de pizza justo antes de acostarse) y la segunda mitad al grupo de control, al que se le indica que no coma nada.

También pueden tomar ciertas medidas para controlar las variables de confusión que pueden producir explicaciones alternativas.

Por ejemplo, al asignar aleatoriamente a los participantes a diferentes grupos, utilizando un método basado en probabilidades, el investigador puede asegurarse de que los participantes coincidan por igual en los posibles factores de confusión, por ejemplo, su historial y predisposición a las pesadillas, el tiempo que les lleva conciliar el sueño e incluso su calidad general del sueño.

Además, el investigador podría llevar a cabo el experimento en un entorno de laboratorio donde se puedan controlar aún más factores, como el entorno del sueño y lo que ven antes de dormir.

Al final, la única forma de establecer la causalidad entre dos variables es mediante la realización de experimentos sonoros.

Key Terms and definitions​

Learning Objectives

Questions that this video will help you answer

This video is also useful for