Waiting
Login processing...

Trial ends in Request Full Access Tell Your Colleague About Jove

Neuroscience

A Low Cost Setup for Behavioral Audiometry in Rodents

Published: October 16, 2012 doi: 10.3791/4433

Summary

A fast and inexpensive method for the behavioral determination of hearing parameters like hearing thresholds, hearing impairments or phantom perceptions (subjective tinnitus) is described. It uses pre-pulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response and can be easily implemented in a personal computer using a programmable AD/DA-converter and a piezo sensor.

Abstract

In auditory animal research it is crucial to have precise information about basic hearing parameters of the animal subjects that are involved in the experiments. Such parameters may be physiological response characteristics of the auditory pathway, e.g. via brainstem audiometry (BERA). But these methods allow only indirect and uncertain extrapolations about the auditory percept that corresponds to these physiological parameters. To assess the perceptual level of hearing, behavioral methods have to be used. A potential problem with the use of behavioral methods for the description of perception in animal models is the fact that most of these methods involve some kind of learning paradigm before the subjects can be behaviorally tested, e.g. animals may have to learn to press a lever in response to a sound. As these learning paradigms change perception itself 1,2 they consequently will influence any result about perception obtained with these methods and therefore have to be interpreted with caution. Exceptions are paradigms that make use of reflex responses, because here no learning paradigms have to be carried out prior to perceptual testing. One such reflex response is the acoustic startle response (ASR) that can highly reproducibly be elicited with unexpected loud sounds in naïve animals. This ASR in turn can be influenced by preceding sounds depending on the perceptibility of this preceding stimulus: Sounds well above hearing threshold will completely inhibit the amplitude of the ASR; sounds close to threshold will only slightly inhibit the ASR. This phenomenon is called pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) 3,4, and the amount of PPI on the ASR gradually depends on the perceptibility of the pre-pulse. PPI of the ASR is therefore well suited to determine behavioral audiograms in naïve, non-trained animals, to determine hearing impairments or even to detect possible subjective tinnitus percepts in these animals. In this paper we demonstrate the use of this method in a rodent model (cf. also ref. 5), the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus), which is a well know model species for startle response research within the normal human hearing range (e.g. 6).

Protocol

1. Setup Assembling and Software Programming

  1. Install a D/A card in a Personal Computer (e.g.: NI PCI 6229, National Instruments) and connect it to a breakout-box (e.g.: BNC-2110, National Instruments), both should support at least one input and one output channel with a sampling rate of at least 44.1 kHz each.
  2. Connect the output of the breakout-box via BNC cable to a sound amplifier (e.g: AMP75 wideband power amplifier, Thomas Wulf).
  3. Install an infrared webcam (e.g.: Grand IP Camera Pro, GrandTec Electronics) for animal surveillance in darkness.
  4. Install an Integrated Development Environment (e.g.: Matlab) to implement a program as defined by the flowchart given in Figure 1. A version running on Matlab 2007b may be obtained free of charge from the corresponding author.
  5. Within a soundproof chamber install a loudspeaker on a table. Connect the speaker to the sound amplifier.
  6. Install a piezo sensor (e.g. force sensor FSG15N1A, Honeywell) on top of an insulating board, support it with power and connect it via BNC cable to the input of the breakout-box; ground the sensor signal.
  7. Build a measuring chamber from an acrylic glass tube adjusted to the size of the rodent to measure (for gerbils e.g.: length 15 cm, inner diameter 4.3 cm, outer diameter 4.8 cm). Fix a grate with a mesh width of 0.5 mm to the front of the tube and a cushioned door with a lock mechanism (e.g., a hook) to the rear. The fixation can be done with hot glue, for the door only the hinge and the bail for the hook is fixed to the tube.
  8. Attach foam plastic feet that fit the measuring tube dimensions on the insulating sensor board. Feet should support the tube under the front and rear end and lift the tube to the sensor level. Make sure that there is light contact between the piezo sensor and the measuring chamber.
  9. Fix the sensor board with the measuring chamber centered in front of the speaker and put a microphone for sound control next to it (e.g. B&K Type 2669 / B&K Type 4190 connected to measuring amplifier B&K Type 2610, all: Bruel and Kjaer) at the level of the animals head so that it is not interfering with the tube.
  10. Note that the quality of your audiometric data will depend on the quality of your sound system. In any case, use the microphone and measuring amplifier to determine the frequency transfer function of your system prior to your first experiments and include a routine in your software to correct for this frequency transfer function to make the spectral output of your speaker flat.
  11. Align the webcam with the setup so that one can monitor animal behavior.

2. Behavioral Determination of Hearing Thresholds (Audiograms)

  1. Take the animal from its home cage and put it head first in the tube; close the door.
  2. Put the tube on the foam feet and the sensor. Switch off any light and close the door to the chamber. The chamber itself is not air conditioned but has the temperature, humidity and other environmental variables of the surrounding laboratory. Air change by ventilators is not advisable due to the induced noise but the volume of the chamber is supporting oxygen for the animal for many hours. Wait 15 min to allow the animal to get accustomed to the setup. The acclimatization time is beneficial for the animal as it can calm down at its own pace and get used to the tube. On the other hand, one does not see any differences in behavior during the acclimatization time over several session, which indicates that an extra session for familiarizing the animal with the tube and the chamber prior to the experiment is not needed.
  3. Start the program and define the parameters for stimulation (cf. also ref. 5): Stimuli consist of pure tones with different frequencies. The startle stimulus must be presented with a sound pressure level high enough to reproducibly elicit a startle response. In our lab we use intensities of 105 dB SPL (stimulus duration 6 ms including 2 ms cosine-squared rise and fall ramps) to elicit startle responses in Mongolian gerbils. The test stimuli preceding the startle stimuli are presented at varying frequencies and intensities in the range to be tested, usually from below hearing threshold to levels well above threshold and with frequencies covering the whole audible range of the species. Frequency and duration of the startle and the test stimulus are matched in each trial, the interstimulus interval between startle and test stimulus is set to 100 ms. Use at least 15 repetitions with randomized interstimulus intervals of 10 ± 2.5 sec for each frequency and intensity combination of the test stimuli (cf. Figure 2A, left). Subsequent test stimuli can be presented in either randomized or non-randomized order. If you are using a non-randomized approach (e.g., one test stimulus level fixed for all tested frequencies) allow 5 min of recovery between different stimulus sets. Note that the determined absolute threshold will depend on the randomization of the stimuli, but possible relative shifts of threshold will not (e.g. after acoustic trauma, cf. ref. 5).
  4. Before analyzing the data, remove invalid trials from dataset (e.g., trials where the animal moved before the startle stimulus; cf. Figure 2B).
  5. Within a time window of the first 50 ms after the startle stimulus, calculate the response amplitude (peak to peak between the first maximum to the first minimum of the response) and response latency (time from stimulus start to response onset) of each single trial.
  6. Fit a Boltzmann-function to the complete response amplitude data set of one frequency sorted for prestimulus intensity for all valid single trials. The 50% point of the Boltzmann-function 7 indicates the hearing threshold for this stimulation frequency.

3. Acoustic Trauma and Quantification of Hearing Impairment

  1. Prepare ketamine-xylazine-anesthesia with a mixture of ketamine hydrochloride: 96 mg/kg (Ketamin-ratiopharm, Ratiopharm); xylazine hydrochloride: 4 mg/kg (Rompun 2%, Bayer); atropine sulfate: 1 mg/kg (Atropinsulfat, B. Braun Melsungen AG) and physiological NaCl-solution (Berlin-Chemie AG, Berlin) with a ratio of 9:1:2:8.
  2. Inject the animal with 3 ml/kg of anesthesia subcutaneous. Wait until the animal is deeply anesthetized (ca. 5 min, check reflexes, e.g., use the pedal withdrawal reflex). To maintain anesthesia during measurements, continuously inject the anesthetic solution at a rate of 3 ml/kg/hr using a syringe pump. Control vital signs with appropriate equipment (e.g., breathing via camera) and keep the animal warm by placing it on a warming pad.
  3. Induce an acoustic trauma, e.g. using a loud pure tone: e.g. 2 kHz at 115dB SPL for 75 min.
  4. After the end of trauma stop the syringe pump and let the animal awake in a wake-up cage on a warming pad at a quiet place. Check regularly during the awakening phase if the vital signs are stable. Put the animal in its home cage only when it is completely awake. Let the animal recover from the anesthesia (at least 2 days) and rest in its home cage.
  5. Perform 2.1 to 2.6 again. Compare the hearing thresholds before and after the acoustic trauma by calculating the percentage of hearing loss for each frequency. After the end of all experiments painlessly euthanize the animal.

4. Test for Acoustic Phantom Perception (Subjective tinnitus)

  1. Perform these measurements before and after the acoustic trauma.
  2. Follow 2.1 to 2.2 if the animal is not already in the setup.
  3. A number of stimulation paradigms can be used to test rodents for subjective tinnitus. The ratio in all these approaches is to test for the salience of a silent gap within a background noise. If the gap is perceived by the animals, it can be used as a test stimulus to reduce a startle response analogously to the procedure described in 2. If the animal suffers from tinnitus (that is likely to develop after acoustic trauma), this tinnitus will be perceived within the silent gap and therefore makes the gap less salient. The effect of the gap on the startle response consequently will be weaker in tinnitus animals compared to healthy controls (reduced PPI, cf. 8). This percept is tested with two slightly different protocols.
  4. In the first subjective tinnitus paradigm presented here (Figure 2A, center, cf. 9) use the following parameters for stimulation: startle sound intensity 105 dB SPL with frequencies from 1 to 16 kHz in 1 octave steps, stimulus length 6 ms including 2 ms cosine-squared rise and fall ramps. Present a white noise background of 50 dB SPL during the experiment, either with or without a 15 ms gap that precedes the startle stimulus by 100 ms; present at least 15 trials for each frequency and gap-condition. If using a non-randomized approach, allow 5 min recovery between the different stimulus sets. Use of different startle stimulus frequencies will give a rough estimate of the perceived tinnitus frequency.
  5. As a second subjective tinnitus paradigm (Figure 2A, right) you may use the following stimulation parameters: startle sound intensity 105 dB SPL, double click stimulus with 0.1 ms duration per click and 0.1 ms between the clicks, the second click having the inverted direction compared to the first. Present a bandpass filtered noise background of 50 dB SPL with a gaussian filter width of 0.5 octaves and center frequencies ranging from 1 to 16 kHz in octave steps. Present this noise either with or without a 15 ms gap that precedes the startle stimulus by 100 ms; present at least 15 trials for each frequency and gap-condition. If using a non-randomized approach, allow 5 min recovery between the different stimulus sets. Use of different center frequencies of the bandpass background noise will give a rough estimate of the perceived tinnitus frequency.
  6. Follow 2.4 to 2.5; normalize all acquired data with a reference data set for each tested frequency and each trauma condition, i.e., before and after an acoustic trauma. This reference is the response amplitude to the pure tone startle stimulus without any prestimulus (cf. 2.3). The frequency is determined either by the pure tone or the center frequency of the bandpass filtered noise. Calculate the mean response of each reference and normalize each calculated response amplitude by dividing it through its reference.
  7. Calculate PPI by dividing the normalized response amplitudes of the gap condition through the mean of the normalized no-gap condition of each tested frequency.
  8. Calculate the PPI change after the trauma in percent for each tested frequency.

The startle responses of animals are easy to generate and to analyze. Figure 2B gives an overview of a typical result of one animal stimulated with a pure tone of 105 dB SPL without any prestimulus for 15 times. The majority of the trials are valid and invalid trials are easy to recognize (trials marked by red square). The response amplitudes and latencies are calculated only from valid trials.

A typical behavioral threshold change is given in Figure 3A. The audiogram of an exemplary animal acquired with the method described in 2 is given before (blue) and after (red) an acoustic trauma at 2 kHz (yellow area). A clear hearing loss is shown specifically at 2 kHz. The responses related to a subjective tinnitus percept can be seen in Figure 3B, the normalized response amplitudes of the same animal as above are shown exemplarily for stimulations one octave below and above the trauma as described in 4.5. The comparison of the responses to stimuli with and without gap before (blue) and after the trauma (red) allows an interpretation of a possible tinnitus percept. Below the trauma frequency no change of response pattern can be found while above the trauma the effect of the gap vanished after the trauma, indicating a mispercept at this frequency.

Figure 1
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the program used to acquire the behavioral thresholds and subjective tinnitus data. Note that this is only a simplified version of the program code. Abbreviations: GUI - graphical user interface; ISI - inter stimulus interval.

Figure 2
Figure 2. Auditory startle response (ASR) stimuli. A Schemes of the three different stimulation protocols used. Left panel: pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) of the ASR measured without any pure tone test stimulus before (green) the startle tone (red); the response period is depicted in blue. Center panel: gap / noise paradigm with pure tone startle stimulus presentation of different frequencies on a white noise background. Right panel: gap / noise paradigm with click startle stimulus presentation on bandpass filtered background of different center frequencies. B Exemplary auditory startle responses of 15 trials recorded with the threshold paradigm without any prestimulus at 1 kHz stimulation frequency. Three trials are counted invalid (red squares) as the animal moved already before the stimulation onset.

Figure 3
Figure 3. Exemplary results of ASR in one animal. A Behavioral threshold before (blue) and after (red) the acoustic trauma at 2 kHz (yellow area). The thresholds are calculated from the responses to the PPI modulated ASR protocol using the Boltzmann-function turning point as threshold value. Note that the hearing loss at 2 kHz amounts to more than 66%, while farer away from the trauma frequency one often can see even improvement of hearing thresholds. B Normalized response amplitudes (open circles: single trials, filled circles: means, whiskers: standard deviation) during stimulation with the gap / noise click ASR protocol (4.5) for 1 and 4 kHz center frequencies. Responses are sorted for trials without and with gap in the noise before and after the trauma at 2 kHz. Only at 4 kHz the effect of the gap vanishes after the trauma which indicates a subjective tinnitus percept around this frequency.

Subscription Required. Please recommend JoVE to your librarian.

Discussion

We present a cheap and easy to build setup for audiometric measurements in rodents based on pre-pulse inhibition of acoustic startle responses that can be used to determine behavioral hearing thresholds (= audiograms 10) and auditory phantom percepts like subjective tinnitus 11. Especially the latter measurements are in the focus of several recent reports 8,12,13,14 and can be seen as one prerequisite for electrophysiological investigations of the neuronal mechanisms underlying this disease. Using this method it is possible differentiate which animals did develop a subjective tinnitus percept after acoustic trauma and those that did not, and then further investigate these individuals, e.g., with electrophysiological recordings in primary auditory cortex.

A critical step in the analysis of the startle data after acoustic trauma is the normalization of the data to the startle amplitude that can maximally be elicited without preceding test stimulus: This is particularly important to distinguish reduced startle responses based on hearing loss from reduced PPI in tinnitus animals: The effects of the acoustic trauma are changing over time, as the animal partially recovers from it, but roughly 50% of the hearing loss is permanent. In contrast to reports mentioned above, where the auditory thresholds are tested but not used for calibration, we tried to minimize the effects of the different hearing thresholds of each frequency and the effect of the acoustic trauma itself by normalizing each response amplitude with a reference. Additionally we use two different kinds of protocols to assess any tinnitus percept, with the first (4.4) working better for animals tested over longer timescales from one week after the trauma on and the second "classical" (4.5) working better for animals tested within one week after the trauma.

A limitation of this method is clearly that one cannot assess the acute effects of an acoustic trauma. At least two days between the anesthesia and the first post-measurement should be chosen, as the animal has to recover from it. To obtain an estimate of acute hearing loss directly after trauma, brainstem audiometry (BERA) may be used.

Subscription Required. Please recommend JoVE to your librarian.

Disclosures

No conflicts of interest declared.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical Research (IZKF, project E7) at the University Hospital of the University of Erlangen-Nuremberg.

References

  1. Brown, M., Irvine, D. R., Park, V. N. Perceptual learning on an auditory frequency discrimination task by cats: association with changes in primary auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 14, 952-965 (2004).
  2. Ohl, F. W., Scheich, H. Learning-induced plasticity in animal and human auditory cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 470-477 (2005).
  3. Koch, M. The neurobiology of startle. Prog. Neurobiol. 59, 107-128 (1999).
  4. Larrauri, J., Schmajuk, N. Prepulse inhibition mechanisms and cognitive processes: a review and model. EXS. 98, 245-278 (2006).
  5. Walter, M., Tziridis, K., Ahlf, S., Schulze, H. Context dependent auditory thresholds determined by brainstem audiometry and prepulse inhibition in Mongolian gerbils. Open Journal of Acoustics. 2, 34-49 (2012).
  6. Gaese, B. H., Nowotny, M., Pilz, P. K. Acoustic startle and prepulse inhibition in the Mongolian gerbil. Physiol. Behav. 98, 460-466 (2009).
  7. Fechter, L. D., Sheppard, L., Young, J. S., Zeger, S. Sensory threshold estimation from a continuously graded response produced by reflex modification audiometry. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 84, 179-185 (1988).
  8. Turner, J. G., Parrish, J. Gap detection methods for assessing salicylate-induced tinnitus and hyperacusis in rats. Am. J. Audiol. 17, 185-192 (2008).
  9. Campeau, S., Davis, M. Fear potentiation of the acoustic startle reflex using noises of various spectral frequencies as conditioned stimuli. Animal Learning & Behavior. 20, 177-186 (1992).
  10. Young, J. S., Fechter, L. D. Reflex inhibition procedures for animal audiometry: a technique for assessing ototoxicity. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 73, 1686-1693 (1983).
  11. Turner, J. G., Brozoski, T. J., Bauer, C. A., Parrish, J. L., Myers, K., Hughes, L. F., Caspary, D. M. Gap detection deficits in rats with tinnitus: a potential novel screening tool. Behav. Neurosci. 120, 188-195 (2006).
  12. Turner, J., Larsen, D. Relationship between noise exposure stimulus properties and tinnitus in rats: Results of a 12-month longitudinal study. ARO. Abs. 594, (2012).
  13. Turner, J. G. Behavioral measures of tinnitus in laboratory animals. Prog. Brain Res. 166, 147-156 (2007).
  14. Engineer, N. D., Riley, J. R., Seale, J. D., Vrana, W. A., Shetake, J. A., Shetake, J. A., Sudanagunta, S. P., Borland, M. S., Kilgard, M. P. Reversing pathological neural activity using targeted plasticity. Nature. 470, 101-104 (2011).

Tags

Behavioral Audiometry Rodents Low Cost Setup Auditory Animal Research Hearing Parameters Brainstem Audiometry Auditory Percept Behavioral Methods Learning Paradigm Lever Press Perception Reflex Response Acoustic Startle Response (ASR) Loud Sounds Naïve Animals
A Low Cost Setup for Behavioral Audiometry in Rodents
Play Video
PDF DOI

Cite this Article

Tziridis, K., Ahlf, S., Schulze, H.More

Tziridis, K., Ahlf, S., Schulze, H. A Low Cost Setup for Behavioral Audiometry in Rodents. J. Vis. Exp. (68), e4433, doi:10.3791/4433 (2012).

Less
Copy Citation Download Citation Reprints and Permissions
View Video

Get cutting-edge science videos from JoVE sent straight to your inbox every month.

Waiting X
Simple Hit Counter