JoVE Science Education
Cognitive Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Science Education Cognitive Psychology
Visual Search for Features and Conjunctions
  • 00:00Overview
  • 00:40Stimulus Design
  • 02:13Conducting the Study
  • 03:04Analysis and Results
  • 04:12Applications
  • 05:05Summary

특징 및 결합에 대한 시각적 검색

English

Share

Overview

출처: 조나단 플롬바움 연구소 -존스 홉킨스 대학

사람들은 어수선한 시각적 장면에서 객체를 어떻게 찾을 수 있습니까? 예를 들어, 지저분한 책상에서 열쇠를 찾고, 식료품점에서 가장 잘 익은 과일을 찾고, 주차한 곳을 기억할 수 없을 때 자동차를 찾거나, 공항 출구 게이트에서 오랜 친구를 찾는 것을 생각해 보십시오. 시각적 인식에 대한 이해는 어떤 답변에도 중요한 역할을 할 것이며, 보다 구체적으로는 시각적 주의에 대한 이해가 중요합니다.

시각적 주의는 이미지의 일부에만 초점을 맞추는 기능을 말하며, 표준 실험 전문 용어에서 찾고 있는 것이 있는지 여부를 결정하기 위해 처리 리소스를 선택적으로 소집합니다. 검색과 주의를 연구하기 위해 실험 심리학자는 시각적 검색으로 널리 사용되는 패러다임을 개발했습니다(당연히).

심리학자들은 또한 어떤 좋은 검색 이론이 있을 것이라는 직관에 의한 많은 연구 동기를 부여했으며, 왜 어떤 것들을 찾기 쉽고 다른 것들은 찾기 어려운지 설명합니다. 그래서 시각적 검색 패러다임의 맥락에서, 지각 심리학자는 종종 더 어려운 것들과 쉽게 검색 대조에 초점을 맞추고있다. 가장 영향력 있는 대조는 연구원이 기능 검색이라고 부르는 것과 연결 검색 사이의 대조입니다.

Procedure

1. 자극 설계 실험에는 두 가지 유형의 시험이 포함됩니다. 시험의 절반에서 기능 검색 시험 – 참가자는 파란색 막대 중 빨간색 막대를 찾아야합니다. 따라서 40개의 디스플레이 세트를 렌더링하여 빨간색 막대를 각각 무작위로 배치하고 3, 6, 9 또는 12개의 파란색 막대도 무작위로 배치합니다. 파란색 막대의 수는 산만 하중입니다. 각 부하에 대해 동일한 수의 시험(이 경우 10개)이 있을 것입…

Results

Note that response times in feature search trials are relatively unaffected by distractor load (Figure 3). In contrast, conjunction search response times increase linearly. In fact, the slope of that function describes the amount of extra search time it takes, on average, for each additional distractor in the scene. In this case, it looks like about 50 ms per item. Similarly, both searches take about 200 ms with only three distractors present. This suggests that a uniform amount of time is necessary to get a search going and make a response.

The difference between feature and conjunction search suggests how one of the challenges faced by the human visual system involves putting different kinds of information together. Finding a red bar amongst all blue ones is easy—it pops out—because only one kind of information is relevant: color. But finding something that is defined by multiple different kinds of features—in this case, orientation and color—needs focused attention to help bind those features together.

Figure 3
Figure 3: Response times as a function of distractor load in target present trials. Feature search and conjunction search conditions are shown in green circles and yellow triangles, respectively.

Applications and Summary

In the real world, understanding how visual search works has many important applications. For example, major research programs are currently applying an understanding of visual search in the laboratory to understand and improve how doctors search for certain telltale signatures when they look at an x-ray or MRI scan. Similar research programs look at how TSA personnel search through scans of passenger baggage at the airport, and even how athletes locate their teammates on a field.

Transcript

Visual attention refers to the ability to focus in on just a part of an image. To study how people attend to objects in cluttered visual scenes, psychologists use a paradigm known as visual search.

Often, visual search experiments can help researchers explain why some objects are easy to find and others more difficult.

Using the visual search paradigm, this video will demonstrate how to design and identify stimuli for experiments, as well as perform, analyze, and interpret results.

To design the stimuli, compose a pair of conditions that are very similar in terms of display contents, but vary in terms of search difficulty. Consider the classic contrasting example between ‘Feature Search’ and ‘Conjunction Search.’

In the Feature Search condition, design trials in which a single feature distinguishes a target amongst its neighbours, known as the distractors. Here, the target is a red bar, and all the distractors are blue bars. The participant should efficiently find the target, as it “pops out” quickly, even when the distractor load increases from three, six, nine, or 12 blue bars.

In the Conjunction Search condition, design trials in which the target shares similarities with distractors. Here, a red target bar is oriented at -45°, and both red and blue distractors are oriented at +45°. In this case, the participant should find the search more difficult because the similarities don’t provide a “pop out” effect.

Within each search condition, create two sets of 40 trials where the target is present or absent. Make sure to include 10 trials with each distractor load of three, six, nine, or 12 bars. Randomly interleave all trials to guarantee unpredictable sequences for each search type.

To begin the experiment, start by running the Feature Search and Conjunction Search tasks. Use a counterbalanced design, so that some participants will begin with Feature Search, whereas others will complete Conjunction Search first.

With the participant sitting at the computer, assign the ‘M’ key to represent target present responses, and the ‘Z’ key for target absent responses. Indicate to the participant that he or she should press the respective keys to complete each trial as quickly as possible, trying not to make mistakes.

During each trial, capture whether the participant’s response was correct or incorrect, as well as the response time. Output the results into a spreadsheet.

After the participant has completed both search types, examine the overall performance for the target absent trials to make sure the participant was paying attention. Exclude any participant who performs less than 75% correct on these trials.

Once criterion performance has been verified, average together each participant’s response times for all of the target present trials, as a function of search condition (Feature vs. Conjunction) and distractor load.

The data are then graphed by plotting the mean response times across distractor loads for the feature and conjunction search conditions. The response times for the Feature Search task are relatively unaffected by distractor load. In contrast, Conjunction Search response times increase linearly with distractor load. In addition, both searches take about 200 ms with the minimal of three distractors present. This suggests that a uniform amount of time is necessary to start searching and make a response.

Now that you are familiar with setting up a visual search experiment, you can apply this approach to answer more specific research questions.

One of the main challenges faced by our visual system involves the complex integration of multiple visual features. Finding a red bar among all blue ones is easy because only color information is relevant.

However, when finding an object that has several different features, such as orientation and color, more attention must be used to bind those features together.

For example, researchers apply visual search properties to improve how physicians search for certain telltale signs when they look at an x-ray or MRI scan.

In addition, the visual search approach affects how TSA personnel search through scans of passenger baggage at the airport.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s introduction on conducting a visual search experiment. Now you should have a good understanding of how to make visual search stimuli for two different types of visual search conditions, how to conduct the experiment, and finally how to analyze and interpret the results.

You should also have an idea about the type of attention that is required when you are looking for keys on a messy desk or finding the ripest-looking fruit at the grocery store.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. Visual Search for Features and Conjunctions. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).