JoVE Science Education
Developmental Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Science Education Developmental Psychology
Memory Development: Demonstrating How Repeated Questioning Leads to False Memories
  • 00:00Overview
  • 01:31Experimental Design
  • 03:35Running the Experiment
  • 04:52Representative Results
  • 05:32Applications
  • 07:26Summary

기억 발달: 반복되는 질문이 어떻게 거짓 기억으로 이어지는 지 시연

English

Share

Overview

출처: 주디스 다노비치와 니콜라우스 놀스 연구소 — 루이빌 대학교

사람은 자신의 삶에서 발생하는 사람과 사건을 기반으로 독특한 개인으로 정의됩니다. 따라서 기억을 만들고, 저장하고, 기억하는 것은 인간의 경험의 필수적인 요소입니다. 그러나, 기억, 성인경험으로, 개발하는 데 시간이 걸립니다. 어린 아이들은 사실을 배우고 일상에서 일상에서 자신의 삶의 세부 사항을 기억할 수 있지만, 3 세 이상까지 자신의 삶에서 일어나는 사건의 자서전 기억이나 상세한 기억을 만들지 않습니다.

3세 이후에도 아이들의 기억은 중요한 면에서 성인의 기억과 다릅니다. 아이들은 성인보다 자신의 기억을 평가하는 데 덜 효과적이며, 예를 들어 기억이 정확한지 여부를 결정하기가 어렵습니다. 거짓 기억은 어린이와 성인 모두에게 문제가되는데, 이는 잘못된 질문이나 이야기가 반복되는 거짓 기억을 만드는 것은 매우 쉽기 때문에 매우 쉽습니다. 그러나 어린 아이들은 나이가 많은 어린이나 성인보다 거짓 기억을 만들기에 더 취약합니다.

이 비디오는 스티븐 세시와 그의 협력자가 개발한 방법을 사용하여 거짓 기억에 대한 아이들의 취약성을 보여줍니다. 1-2

Procedure

발달 장애의 병력이없는 약 50 명의 건강한 5-6 세를 모집하십시오. 이 데모를 위해 한 명의 어린이만 테스트됩니다. 실험을 수행할 때 더 큰 샘플 크기를 권장합니다. 샘플 크기가 큰 것은 참가자의 감소 또는 손실을 고려하는 것입니다. 이 절차는 어린이들이 특정 시간 동안 특정 횟수를 완료하도록 요구하는 세로다중 세션 디자인을 사용합니다. 세션을 놓친 모든 어린이는 데…

Results

Children presented with real events and unrealistic events from their lives are typically very accurate at identifying situations they have and have not experienced. However, when presented with realistic events they have not experienced, many preschoolers believe they experienced those events, even after only hearing about them once, and the rate at which they say they have experienced them increases over time (Figure 1). In addition to the data from children’s responses, children may also spontaneously add their own details to the false memories presented to them in earlier testing sessions.

Figure 1
Figure 1: Average percentage of children who say they experienced an event that happened to them (true memory) or did not happen to them (false memory) after one, three, or five sessions.

Applications and Summary

Memories are not perfect representations of life events. They degrade over time, and details can be added or subtracted. Remembering is more like construction than recollection. So, it is very easy for a person to mistake something they have heard before, or something they have thought about before, for something that they’ve actually experienced. This is especially true for children, who are particularly likely to form false memories when asked to think about or imagine situations or events. More generally, these findings indicate that people should take special care when questioning children about serious legal and personal matters. Children are typically questioned repeatedly and with very specific questions when they are interviewed, and parents, teachers, social workers, and law enforcement officials commonly employ these practices. Thus, there is a delicate balance between creating false memories and effectively provoking children to report what they know about important events.

References

  1. Ceci, S.J., & Bruck, M. Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scientific analysis of children’s testimony. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (1995).
  2. Ceci, S.J., Crotteau Huffman, M.L., Smith, E., & Loftus, E.F. Repeatedly thinking about a non-event: Source misattributions among preschoolers. Consciousness and Cognition. 3, 388-407 (1994).

Transcript

Both children and adults can experience false memories, which are either memories of events that never happened, or real memories that have been altered by subsequent experiences.

Compared to adults, children are more susceptible to forming false memories. Often, such memories are harmless cases of children recalling events and experiences that never actually occurred—like going to the hospital the day their sibling was born when they actually spent the day with their grandparents.

In addition, when children remember, it can be challenging for them to separate real details and events from those that they imagined or only heard about. As a result, children are less adept than adults at judging the accuracy of their memories.

As the work of psychologist Stephen Ceci has revealed, one of the most worrisome aspects of false memories is that they can be easily created in children, for example by repeatedly asking them a question or telling them a story.

Using the techniques described by Ceci and colleagues, this video demonstrates how to design, collect, and interpret data for an experiment investigating false memories in children, as well as how to apply this method to explore the complex relationship between memory, imagination, and age.

In this experiment, 5- and 6-year-olds are told about different events involving them, and asked to remember whether they experienced these scenarios.

The scenarios consist of three types: true, check, and test events. True events are those that children were actually involved in—like having gone to the fair and winning an enormous stuffed animal—that a researcher identifies by interviewing the children’s parents or guardians beforehand.

In this case, the dependent variable is the average percentage of children that assented, or claimed to have actually experienced, true events. These events are also used to evaluate the general accuracy of children’s memory.

The second type, check events, are fantastical scenarios a researcher creates that children did not experience, for example, having played a board game with a talking dog by a roaring fire.

As these events couldn’t have happened, they ensure that children don’t get into the habit of automatically answering “Yes” to every question, and also evaluate memory accuracy.

The final type of events—test events—are the trickiest, and consist of plausible scenarios that children did not experience, such as going to see a doctor after stepping on broken glass.

The percentage of children who claimed to have experienced test events serves as the dependent variable.

Over a 5-week period, children are repeatedly asked about the same set of scenarios, which includes true, check, and test events.

Based on the previous work by Ceci and colleagues, it is expected that the percentage of children assenting to test events will increase over questioning sessions, indicating that in some children these scenarios form false memories.

To prepare for the experiment, confer with every child’s parents to identify events that they have and haven’t experienced over the last year.

Using this information, design a collection of 10 individualized vignettes on index cards for each child. Ensure that each set includes five true, three check, and two test events.

To begin the study, greet the child and introduce them to the task they will be performing. Emphasize that some of the events they will be told about didn’t actually happen.

Sit nearby while holding index cards on which events are printed.

Have the child select a card at random, and read to them what’s written on it. Afterwards, ask the child whether the event happened.

Continue until all the cards have been read, and transcribe the child’s responses. Repeat this procedure five times over a period of five weeks.

For each of these five sessions, code the child’s “yes” responses separately for true events and test events.

To analyze the data, calculate the average percentage of children who claimed to have experienced both true and test events across sessions 1, 3, and 5.

Perform an analysis of variance to determine if there are differences between the two types of events presented to children in these three sessions.

Notice the rate at which children claim to have experienced realistic test events increased over time, indicating that many of them come to believe these events actually happened—forming false memories.

Now that you know how repeated questioning can be used to investigate false memories in children, let’s look at how psychologists are studying the plasticity of memory in other contexts.

One application of this work evaluates whether repeated questioning by authority figures—such as law enforcement officials—during interviews can result in children forming false memories.

As this can have serious legal ramifications, many psychologists are looking to identify and develop questioning strategies that encourage children to accurately report what they know about an event without influencing their memory of it.

Similarly, psychologists are trying to understand how children can mistake something they’ve heard, imagined, or thought about for something that they’ve actually experienced.

This may be due to the fact that each time a memory—like a specific birthday party—is recalled, there are opportunities for details to be added to or subtracted from it—like who was there and what food was served. Thus, remembering is more like construction than recollection.

As a result, unlike photographs, over time memories can become increasingly imperfect representations of a life event.

Finally, some researchers use techniques—like functional MRI—that can identify metabolically active tissues to compare areas of the brain involved in the formation and retrieval of false and true memories.

Although individuals experience both true and false memories in much the same way, it is possible that these imaging techniques can identify structures in the brain—like the hippocampus—that may help differentiate between these types of memories.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on the development of false memories in children. By now, you should understand how repeated questioning can lead to false memories in children, and know how to collect and interpret data revealing children’s tendencies to form false memories. You should also have an idea of what makes children particularly susceptible to false memories.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. Memory Development: Demonstrating How Repeated Questioning Leads to False Memories. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).