JoVE Science Education
Developmental Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Science Education Developmental Psychology
Mutual Exclusivity: How Children Learn the Meanings of Words
  • 00:00Overview
  • 01:12Experimental Design
  • 02:06Running the Experiment
  • 02:44Representative Results
  • 03:20Applications
  • 04:52Summary

互斥: 儿童如何学习的单词的意思

English

Share

Overview

资料来源: 实验室的尼古拉 Noles 和朱迪思 Danovitch — — 路易斯维尔大学

人类是不同于其他动物的很多方面,但最重要的差异化因素也许是他们使用语言的能力。其他动物可以沟通,甚至理解和使用语言,以有限的方式,但试图教给黑猩猩或一只狗的人类语言需要大量的时间和精力。与此相反的是,年轻人容易,掌握自己的母语和他们学习语言规则没有明确的指示,是一个甚至是最聪明的动物无法比拟的成就。

年轻的人类对动物的一个优点是,人类的大脑是特别适合于学习新单词。只有几次接触,年轻子女可以学习新单词,并记住它们。也许更令人印象深刻的是,孩子们可以使用他们已经知道指导他们今后的学习。例如,儿童治疗对象,好像他们有只有一个标签。所以,如果一个孩子学会了词锤,他们不会承担不熟悉的工具具有相同的名称。这是相互排斥性的原则。1-2

该视频演示使用互斥以匹配对象在其环境中的单词儿童的能力。

Procedure

招募健康 2 岁儿童正常听力与视觉和发育障碍没有历史。对于本演示的目的,测试只有一个孩子。更大的样本量被建议时进行任何实验。 1.收集必要的材料。 选择一个熟悉而又陌生的测试对象。 熟悉测试对象应该是可识别大多数 2 岁。在这种情况下,使用一只玩具香蕉。 不熟悉测试对象应该是大多数 2 岁还不熟悉的东西。在这种情况下,使用压蒜器…

Results

Given two objects, each child responding at random would have a 50% chance of grabbing the unfamiliar object first. However, if the child knows the label of the familiar object and treats it as being exclusive to that object, then they should guess that the new label refers to the unfamiliar object (Figure 1). Because 2-year-olds have different experiences, not every child knows or remembers the label for the banana. So some toddlers select the banana, but most link the unfamiliar object to the novel label. In order to have enough power to see significant results, researchers would have to test at least 18 children. 

Figure 1
Figure 1: Pie chart showing the percent of children who selected the unfamiliar object.

Applications and Summary

The world is full of objects, and one of the early challenges faced by children who are learning a language is to match the labels they hear to the correct objects in their environment. Children have several tendencies that help them to solve this problem. First, they treat labels as referring to whole objects, so they don’t get confused about what is being labeled. For example, when a child hears “banana,” they don’t think the label is a feature of the object, like a part or a color; they assume the word refers to the whole object. Second, children treat these labels as being exclusive. So, each object has only one name. Thus, if they hear a new label, they can assume it does not apply to any of the many items they’ve already learned to name. Since children learn words very effectively during this time in their development, they quickly reduce the ambiguity in their environment, and the problem of linking labels to objects becomes increasingly easy to solve using the principle of mutual exclusivity.

The finding that children can determine the meaning of a new word without having to receive direct instruction from another person is important, because it shows that parents and other adults do not have to make a special effort to teach their child language. Instead, just by speaking naturally and introducing the child to a wide range of objects and experiences, adults are actually enabling the child to learn new vocabulary.

References

  1. Markman, E.M., & Wachtel, G.F. Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology. 20, 121-157 (1988).
  2. Merriman, W.E., & Bowman, L.L. The mutual exclusivity bias in children’s word learning. Mongraphs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 54 (Serial Nos. 3-4) (1989).

Transcript

Children acquire their native language rapidly and learn linguistic rules without explicit instruction.

During development, the brain is especially adapted to learn new words, giving young children an advantage. After only a few exposures to novel words, young children learn and remember them effortlessly.

Perhaps more impressively, children can use what they already know to guide their future learning. For example, if a child has learned the word hammer, they will assume that an unfamiliar tool has a different name. This is the principle of mutual exclusivity—the assumption that objects only have one label.

Based on the research of Drs. Markman and Wachtel, this video demonstrates how to setup and test children’s ability to use mutual exclusivity during early word learning, as well as how to analyze and interpret data for matching words to objects in their environment.

In this experiment, 2 year-old children will be asked to distinguish a familiar object, for example, a banana from an unfamiliar object, such as a garlic press.

Specifically, each child is told that one of the objects is called a dax—a name that the researcher made up—while the familiar item is not mentioned. They are then asked to choose which object is the dax.

If the child doesn’t understand mutual exclusivity, they will respond randomly and be equally likely to pick either object. However, if the label of the familiar object is treated as being mutually exclusive, then the child will guess that the new label refers to the unfamiliar object and pick it more often.

To begin the experiment, greet the child and instruct them to sit in a chair while you sit in the other one facing them. Place two items—a familiar banana and an unfamiliar garlic press—on the table out of reach of the child.

Explain to the child: “Look what I have. I have a dax here.” Simultaneously push the objects closer to the child and ask: “Can you get the dax?”

Make a note of which object the child picks as being the dax.

To analyze the results, count the number of children that picked the unfamiliar and familiar objects as corresponding to the new label dax, and graph the percentages as a pie chart.

Note that most children used mutual exclusivity and linked the unfamiliar object to the novel label. However, some 2-year-olds have different experiences, so not every child knows or remembers the label for banana, which led some children to select the banana.

Now that you are familiar with how young children match words to objects, let’s look at how mutual exclusivity helps children learn the meanings of words.

One of the most important aspects of mutual exclusivity is that children can determine the meaning of words efficiently since they do not need direct instructions from adults. This means that just by speaking naturally and introducing children to a wide range of objects and experiences, adults enable children to learn new vocabulary.

In addition, children learn the names of parts of an object faster when the object’s name has already been established through mutual exclusivity. For example, if a child knows that an object is a bull, then they can use mutual exclusivity to conclude that unfamiliar words referring to the bull must reference its parts instead of the whole animal.

Similarly, knowing the names of some of the parts can help children to identify exactly what one is being referenced, and to link a new word with the appropriate part. In this way, every word that a child learns makes them a better and more efficient learner.

Perhaps the most interesting aspect of mutual exclusivity is that this process is effortless for most children. The child simply looks, listens, and experiences the world, and their brain matches words to objects and organizes the world around them.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s introduction to mutual exclusivity. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and run an experiment investigating how children match words to objects, as well as how to analyze and assess the results.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. Mutual Exclusivity: How Children Learn the Meanings of Words. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).