JoVE Science Education
Cognitive Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Science Education Cognitive Psychology
Approximate Number Sense Test
  • 00:00Overview
  • 01:03Experimental Design
  • 02:01Running the Experiment
  • 03:12Representative Results
  • 03:32Applications
  • 03:56Summary

대략적인 숫자 감지 테스트

English

Share

Overview

출처: 조나단 플롬바움 연구소 -존스 홉킨스 대학

일반적인 카니발 게임은 항아리에 포장 된 젤리빈의 수를 추측하는 사람들에게 요청하는 것입니다. 누구든지 정확한 숫자를 올바르게 얻을 가능성이 낮습니다. 그러나 누군가가 17 또는 147,000을 추측 할 가능성은 어떻습니까? 아마도 정답을 추측 할 수있는 기회보다 훨씬 적습니다. 17과 147,000은 비이성적인 것처럼 보입니다. 왜? 결국, 콩을 꺼내서 한 번에 하나씩 계산할 수 없다면, 누군가가 추정치가 너무 높거나 너무 낮다는 것을 어떻게 알 수 있습니까?

그것은 구두 계산 에 추가 (뭔가 명확 하 게 배운), 사람들이 숫자를 추정 하기 위한 하드 와이어 정신 및 신경 메커니즘을 가지고 나타납니다. 구어체로 말하자면, 그것은 추측 할 수있는 능력, 또는 “야구장”이라고 할 수 있습니다. 실험 심리학자들은 그것을 “대략적인 숫자 감각”이라고 부르며, 같은 이름의 실험 패러다임이있는 최근 연구는 추측 할 수있는 능력을 지원하는 기본 계산 및 신경 메커니즘을 밝히기 시작했습니다.

이 비디오는 대략적인 숫자 감지 테스트를 통해 비언어적 수치 추정을 조사하기 위한 표준 절차를 보여줍니다.

Procedure

1. 자극과 시험 Psychopy, MATLAB 또는 유사한 것(www.panamath.org 비상업적 용도로도 무료 버전을 다운로드할 수 있음)에서실험을 프로그래밍합니다. 실험의 모든 시험을 설계하여 더 많거나 적게 동일하게 보입니다. 디스플레이를 반으로 나눕니다. 회색 배경을 사용합니다. 디스플레이의 한쪽에는 파란색 원 컬렉션이 표시됩니다. 다른 쪽…

Results

To graph the results from a participant, average performance as a function of the ratio on each trial (Figure 2). For example, across all 20 trials with a ratio of 2:1, in what fraction did the participant supply the right answer?

Figure 2
Figure 2. Sample results from a single participant in the approximate number test. Performance, measured as response accuracy, increases as the ratio difference between the larger and smaller set of dots increases. Since the participant makes a binary choice—yellow or blue bigger—chance is 50%.

Performance, measured as response accuracy, increases as the ratio difference between the larger and smaller set of dots increases. Since the participant makes a binary choice—yellow or blue bigger—chance is 50%. Note that the participant’s performance improves as the ratio difference increases. But the function is not linear, since there is a ceiling of 100% on how well one can do. The fact that performance is ratio-constrained suggests that numerical approximation is controlled by an analog or magnitude-like mechanism. An analogy is useful here. Imagine representing two quantities by dropping a fistful of sand into a bucket for each dot seen, one bucket for yellow dots and one for blue dots. It is very unlikely that you would deposit the same amount of sand in the buckets on each drop. So say one bucket represents four dots—it has four handfuls of sand in it. And the other represents eight dots—it has eight handfuls of sand. You could weigh the buckets, and easily know which was meant to represent more dots. But now imagine that the larger bucket was only meant to represent five dots—it has only five handfuls of sand it. It will probably still weigh more than the bucket with four, but not by a lot. And because you may sometimes grab a little more sand, and sometimes a little less, there might even be occasions where the bucket meant to represent four ends up weighing more! This is an analog system. The representation—in this case, sand mass—does a good job of capturing large proportional differences between represented quantities, but because of noise, small differences can be hard to tell apart.

The result is that such systems are ratio-constrained. The ability to tell apart more or less depends on the ratio difference between the quantities, not the subtractive difference. It’s as easy to tell apart eight and four as it is eight and sixteen. On the other hand, eight versus twelve is more difficult, even though it subtracts to a difference of four as well.

Applications and Summary

People differ between one another considerably in terms of the acuity of their approximate number sense. To characterize differences between individuals, experimental psychologists generally test to find the smallest ratio a person can tell apart with 75% accuracy. As shown in Figure 2, it is a ratio somewhere between 1.25 and 1.5. This number is just a quick way of summarizing how acute an approximate number sense a person has. But beyond the fact that there are large differences between people—one person might have a ratio of 1:1 and another might have a ratio of 1:4, for example—these differences correlate significantly with formal math ability. For instance, 75%-correct ratios in young children correlate with arithmetic abilities as measured by standardized tests. This is surprising, because ultimately, arithmetic is not about estimating. However, these kinds of correlations suggest that formal math ability depends on an underlying approximate number sense.

Transcript

The approximate number sense test is an experimental paradigm for investigating the underlying mechanisms that support the ability to “guesstimate.”

Guesstimating refers to an intuitive ability to recognize quantity, without knowing the exact number. For instance, in a common carnival game, individuals try to guess the number of jellybeans packed into a jar. The chances are low that anyone will pick the exact number.

Yet everyone can produce a guess in the right ballpark, as no one would guess 20 when there are clearly more than 100. Therefore, estimation is considered a hard-wired ability that individuals possess without relying on mathematical calculations.

This video demonstrates the procedure for investigating nonverbal numerical estimation, including how to design the stimuli, perform the experiment, and how to analyze and interpret data.

In this experiment, stimuli that vary in size and color are randomly and briefly presented on a computer screen. During each trial, two sets are visible: one contains a collection of blue circles, and the other includes a set of yellow circles.

Participants are asked to guess which set contains more. The dependent variable is percent accuracy, or the number of correct responses recorded as a function of the ratios across trials.

Performance accuracy is expected to be near chance when the ratio of circles are very similar—near 1:1—and improve as the ratio differences increase.

In other words, it’s easier to tell apart eight and four versus twelve and eight. In both cases, the subtractive difference is four, but the ratio differences vary, from 2:1 to 1.5:1.

To create the stimuli, generate circles of various sizes in blue and yellow sets. For each set, make sure that the numbers of blue and yellow circles are always different and represent the six ratios.

For each trial, code the program to divide the display to show one set from each color group on a gray background for 500 ms. Note that the color and circle size for the larger amount should be selected at random, and 20 trials with each ratio should be produced.

To begin the experiment, greet the participant in the lab and explain the instructions for the task. Once the participant understands the task rules, load the program.

When the circles disappear in each trial, have the participant press the ‘Y’ key if they think they saw more yellow dots, or the ‘B’ key if they think they saw more blue dots.

After each trial, provide immediate feedback via a tone to indicate whether the participant’s response was correct or incorrect.

To analyze the data, average the number of correct responses as a function of the ratio on each trial. Graph the mean accuracy percentage across ratio differences. Note that participants’ performances improved as the ratio differences increased.

Approximate number sense positively correlates with arithmetic abilities as measured by standardized tests, even though arithmetic is not about estimating.

In addition, even young children can apply number sense to identify when something is missing from a group of familiar objects.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s introduction to the Approximate Number Sense Test. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and perform the experiment, as well as analyze results and apply the phenomenon of number estimation.

Thanks for watching! 

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. Approximate Number Sense Test. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).