JoVE Science Education
Developmental Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Science Education Developmental Psychology
Using Your Head: Measuring Infants’ Rational Imitation of Actions
  • 00:00Overview
  • 01:14Experimental Design
  • 02:47Running the Experiment
  • 06:07Representative Results
  • 06:43Applications
  • 07:46Summary

用你的头: 测量婴儿的理性模仿的行动

English

Share

Overview

资料来源: 实验室的朱迪思 · Danovitch 和尼古拉 Noles — — 路易斯维尔大学

婴儿期的主要挑战之一是学会如何去实现自己的目标,在世界上,它们是否拿了一个玩具或表达欲望给另一个人,在这个学习过程中的最强大工具之一就是模仿。然而,模仿并不总是那么简单复制其他人的行动;它也是需要实现的目标和意图指导行为。世界是一个复杂的地方,和婴儿模仿的行动不完美的呈现。例如,考虑宝宝,看着他们的父亲从塑料瓶水喝。当他拿起瓶子,他不小心扔在地板上。他捡起了和粉尘它关闭之前打开瓶盖,喝了一。如果宝宝想要喝从上他们自己的瓶子,他们不得不解码这套复杂的事件,并确定哪些操作,他们的目标与有关。他们必须把它和它的灰尘,或者他们只需拧紧盖子,并喝一口吗?

为了解决这个问题的一种方法是查看的相同的行为,很多例子但是有些行为是罕见或不同每次他们都执行。因此,它是有益于婴儿再多想想他们是模仿,而不是他们所观察的具体情况的人。如果婴儿假设人是理性的有他们有效地追求的目标,他们可以观察复杂的事件和独立的面向目标的措施,从附带或无关他们的目标的行动。分析一个人的行为的原因允许婴儿来决定是否有意义模仿那人的具体行动,或在一个简单或更有效的方式做事情。

本实验演示如何测量婴儿的理性模仿的演员使用由 Meltzoff1和盖伊,Bekkering 和解围的方法。2

Procedure

招聘 14 个月大的婴儿的数目。参与者应是没有历史的发育障碍,有正常的听力和视力。 因为这个年龄的婴儿可以不合作或模糊 (例如,拒绝观看演示) 和这个实验需要两届会议,预计招聘和测试额外参与者获得充分的数据。 1.数据收集 会议一 触摸灯 (至少在直径 6) 置于框中或低的平台,在一个实验者在哪里可以找到它与他们的额…

Results

Researchers tested 27 infants and found that 69% of the infants in the hands-free condition re-enacted the head action during session two. In contrast, only 21% of the infants in the hands-occupied condition did so (Figure 1). This supports the claim that 14-month-olds are able to evaluate the reasons for an adult’s behavior. When they viewed the experimenter touching the lamp with their head while their hands were occupied, they inferred that they would have used their hands if they were free, and since their own hands were free, they completed the action in the simplest way possible. However, when the experimenter’s hands were free, and they used their head to turn on the lamp, infants inferred there must be a reason for their unusual behavior and were more likely to imitate their actions. This shows that infants imitate selectively, based on their understanding that adults act in a rational, goal-directed manner.

Figure 1
Figure 1: Percentage of participants in each condition who used their head or their hands only to turn on the lamp..

Applications and Summary

Imitation is crucial for learning about a wide range of human behaviors, ranging from language to tool use. However, the ability to rationally imitate actions shows that even infants do not simply copy what they see other people do. Instead, by age 1, children are already thinking about the reasons for a person’s behavior and using that to guide their own behaviors—even if a long time has passed since they first observed the person’s behavior.

Rational imitation can be applied to understanding the development of creativity and flexibility in problem solving. In real life, it does not always make sense for a person to do things only the way they have seen them done before. Instead, it is important to analyze the constraints on the situation and try to address the problem as efficiently as possible. In addition, this work has educational applications. It shows that even very young children can learn and remember actions they have observed, and they do not require extensive practice or reminders to repeat those actions.

Developing an understanding of rational behavior may also have broader consequences than simply learning how to effectively imitate. Take, for example, slapstick comedy. Seeing an actor slip on a banana peel because it was their goal to do so is not funny. However, the same actor accidentally slipping on a banana peel in a way not aligned with their goals is hysterical. Thus, understanding rational behavior may have consequences that go beyond imitation and into other areas of social-cognitive development.

References

  1. Meltzoff, A. N. Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology. 24 (4), 470 (1988).
  2. Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature. 415 (6873), 755-755 (2002).

Transcript

Imitation is a form of social learning that goes beyond simply copying others’ actions and involves understanding the goals and intentions behind behaviors.

For example, think of an infant watching their father drink from a water bottle. If the father accidentally drops the bottle while the lid is closed before taking a drink, the infant must then decode this complex set of events to decide which steps are necessary, and which can be ignored, to simply drink water.

Ultimately, once infants assume that others are rational, they can understand which goal-oriented actions to imitate exactly and which incidental actions to ignore, such as drinking from the water bottle without first dropping it on the floor.

This video demonstrates how to design and conduct an imitation experiment in infants based on the seminal methods of Andrew Meltzoff, György Gergely, and colleagues, as well as how to analyze and interpret results across rational and irrational conditions.

In this 2-group experiment, 14-month-old infants are assigned to one of two conditions—one in which an adult’s hands are occupied or one where they are free.

In the hands-occupied condition, infants evaluate a rational scenario, where an adult pretends to be cold, wraps a blanket around themselves, and keeps both hands holding the blanket. They are logically unable to touch a lamp with their hands and must do so with their head.

Alternatively, in the hands-free condition, infants observe an irrational situation. In this case, the adult again pretends to be cold and uses the blanket, but then rests both hands flat on the table, clearly making their hands available to use. However, the experimenter still touches the lamp with their head.

In a follow-up session one week later, the infant is simply placed near the same touch lamp and briefly observed over a short, 20-s period.

During this observation period, the dependent variable is recorded as the percentage of infants who mimic the adult by touching the lamp with their head or respond by using their hands.

It is hypothesized that young infants imitate selectively. That is, when they infer that there is a reason for the unusual behavior they are observing—as in the hands-free condition—they mimic exact actions rather than responding simply by using their hands.

Prior to the experimental sessions, gather the necessary materials, which include a touch-sensitive lamp at least 6 in. in diameter, a platform to rest the lamp on, a few small toys, and a blanket.

For session one, place the touch sensitive lamp on the platform at a height where you can reach it with your forehead.

Then, set up a rectangular table with two chairs on one side—one for you and one for the parent. Move the lamp out of sight but within reach, and place some toys on the table.

To begin the session, invite the parent and infant into the room. Direct them to sit on the chair to the left with the infant on their lap, while you sit on the chair to the right.

Help the infant become comfortable with the environment by providing some toys for them to play with for 1-3 min.

While the infant is playing, inform the parent that you will be demonstrating an action for the infant, and that they must avoid interacting with their child during this time.

Following the explanation, put away the toys, and place the touch lamp on the platform.

Start the experiment by getting the infant’s attention. After randomly dividing them into one of two conditions, pretend to be cold and wrap a blanket around you. Continue to hold the blanket with both hands for the hands-occupied group versus visibly resting both hands flat on the table in the other hands-free condition.

Next, lean forward, touch the lamp with your forehead, and then straighten back to an upright position; repeat this procedure 3x in a 20-s period. Note that the same action is performed for both groups, only the hand positions are different.

Following the demonstration, escort the parent and infant from the testing room and inform the parent not to discuss or model the actions shown to their child.

Seven days later, prepare for session two by setting up a video camera to record the infant’s torso, head, and the tabletop. Also, verify that the lamp is turned off to prevent it from accidentally illuminating.

After starting the video recording, escort the parent and infant into the testing room and have them sit at the table as they previously did during the first session.

Once again, provide toys for the infant to play with for 1-3 min to acclimate them to the environment.

Then, remove the toys and place the touch lamp on the table directly in front of the infant. Allow them to interact with the lamp, and after 20 s from the initial contact, stop the recording.

When the study is finished, have two independent raters, who are blind to the conditions, code the 20-s interaction period for each infant by writing a yes response if the infant made head contact or leaned very close to the lamp, or no if they did not.

To analyze the responses, compare the percentage of infants that used their hands only, or their head, to touch the lamp.

Note that 69% of infants in the hands-free condition re-enacted the head action during session two. In contrast, only 21% of infants in the hands-occupied condition did so. These data support the claim that 14-month-olds are able to evaluate the reasons for an adult’s behavior well after they first observed the behavior.

Now that you are familiar with how infants are already thinking about the reasons for a person’s behavior and using them to guide their own behaviors, you can apply how such rational imitation is critical in a number of situations.

Understanding rational behavior underlies many slapstick comedy routines. For instance, seeing an actor slip on a banana peel because it was their goal to do so is not funny. However, the same actor accidentally slipping on a banana peel in a way not aligned with their goals is hysterical.

Rational imitation can also be applied to understanding the development of creativity and flexibility in problem solving.

Performing tasks the way that individuals have seen them done before, like standing on a wobbly chair to reach a fan switch, is not always the most efficient—or safest—solution. Rather, with a little ingenuity, a new solution can be developed.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s introduction to studying rational imitation in infants. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and conduct the experiment, and finally how to analyze and interpret the results.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. Using Your Head: Measuring Infants’ Rational Imitation of Actions. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).