JoVE Science Education
Neuropsychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Science Education Neuropsychology
Learning and Memory: The Remember-Know Task
  • 00:00Overview
  • 01:21Experimental Design
  • 03:24Running the Experiment
  • 06:25Data Analysis and Representative Results
  • 08:38Applications
  • 10:42Summary

학습과 기억: 기억-알기 과제

English

Share

Overview

출처: 조나스 T. 카플란과 사라 I. 짐벨의 연구소 – 서던 캘리포니아 대학

우리의 기억 경험은 다양하고 복잡합니다. 때때로 우리는 사건을 생생하게 기억하고, 다른 때에는 모호한 친숙함만 있을 수 있습니다. 메모리 연구원은 익숙한 기억 대 회수 기억 사이 구별을 만들었습니다. 회수된 항목은 기억될 뿐만 아니라 학습또는 인코딩된 시간의 세부 정보를 담고 있습니다. 회수된 항목과 마찬가지로 친숙한 항목도 기억되지만 인코딩을 둘러싼 상황에 대한 세부 사항도 없습니다. 기억과 친숙함의 많은 연구는 기억 인코딩, 통합 및 검색에 관여하기 때문에 내측 측두엽 (MTL) 특히 해마에 초점을 맞추고 있습니다. 1-3

이 비디오는 기억-알고 작업을 관리 하는 방법을 보여줍니다4 메모리 검색의이 두 가지 유형에서 뇌 활성화를 비교. 이러한 맥락에서 기억은 기억에 남는 또 다른 용어이지만, 아는 것은 친숙하지만 명시적으로 회수되지 않은 기억을 가리킵니다. 기억-알고 있는 작업의 이 버전에서는 참가자들이 일련의 컬러 이미지에 노출되어 표시되는 내용을 기억하도록 요청합니다. fMRI 스캐너 내부에서는 연구된 이미지와 새로운 이미지모두에 노출되며 각 이미지에 대해 “기억”, “알고” 또는 “새로운” 판단을 내릴 것이며, 이는 해당 항목에 대해 어떤 종류의 메모리가 있는지 를 나타냅니다. 검사 후, 전체 뇌 및 해마 활동은 기억과 친숙함과 관련된 차동 활성을 결정하기 위해 검사될 것입니다. 이 연구는 김벨과 브루어가 수행한 연구를 기반으로 합니다. 5

Procedure

1. 참가자 모집 20명의 참가자를 모집합니다. 참가자는 오른손잡이여야 하며 신경학적 또는 심리적 장애의 병력이 없어야 합니다. 참가자는 시각적 신호를 제대로 볼 수 있도록 정상 또는 수정된 투 정상 시야를 가져야 합니다. 참가자는 몸에 금속이 없어야 합니다. 이것은 fMRI에 관련된 높은 자기장 때문에 중요한 안전 요구 사항입니다. fMRI는 스캐너 보어의 ?…

Results

Regions more active for remember responses than for know responses are shown in Figure 1. Notably, the hippocampus, a structure located in the MTL and known to be involved in many stages of memory formation and retrieval, showed greater activity for remember compared with know trials.

Figure 1
Figure 1: Cluster maps of Remember minus Know. Hippocampus is outlined in yellow. Clusters are overlaid on an average anatomical brain of the study participants (p < 0.01, corrected for multiple comparisons). Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Inspection of the time-course of activity in the hippocampus (Figure 2) shows that this structure is selectively responding when participants report explicitly remembering the stimuli, and is not responding when they only have feelings of familiarity, or when they do not remember the stimuli at all.

Figure 2
Figure 2. Hippocampal activity over time. Each line shows activity in the hippocampus over the course of trials of each type. "Remember" and "Know" are trials in which participants correctly reported remembering the stimuli. "Miss" trials refer to stimuli that were presented before but not correctly remembered by the participant. "Correct Rejections" are new stimuli that participants correctly identified as new. Y-axis is percent signal change from baseline; X-axis is time (s) after the onset of the stimulus.

These results suggest that the hippocampus is involved in the process of memory retrieval, but that it does not contribute to feelings of familiarity, supporting a dual-process theory. According to this view, a second cognitive process, one that does not depend on the hippocampus, generates familiarity. However, in the Remember-Know task, memory strength may be confounded with memory type. In other words, it is possible that hippocampal activity is greater for remember trials because those memories are stronger, and not because they are qualitatively different from know trials. To distinguish between these explanations, memory strength would have to be equated across trial types.

Applications and Summary

This experiment demonstrates how cognitive neuroscientists attempt to tease apart the specific contributions of a brain region to a cognitive task. Isolating subtle variations within a cognitive domain, in this case the different subjective experiences associated with memory retrieval, can reveal dissociations in the neural systems that support those functions. Understanding how the brain functions during different types of memory retrieval is important for understanding memory impairments such as those that result from traumatic brain injury or from degenerative diseases. Furthermore, an understanding of the cognitive neuroscience of memory retrieval may also inform strategies for improving memory.

References

  1. Bayley, P.J. & Squire, L.R. Failure to acquire new semantic knowledge in patients with large medial temporal lobe lesions. Hippocampus 15, 273-280 (2005).
  2. Cohen, N.J. & Squire, L.R. Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science 210, 207-210 (1980).
  3. Scoville, W.B. & Milner, B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 20, 11-21 (1957).
  4. Yonelinas, A.P. Components of episodic memory: the contribution of recollection and familiarity. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 356, 1363-1374 (2001).
  5. Gimbel, S.I. & Brewer, J.B. Reaction time, memory strength, and fMRI activity during memory retrieval: Hippocampus and default network are differentially responsive during recollection and familiarity judgments. Cogn Neurosci 2, 19-23 (2011).

Transcript

Our experience of memory is varied and complex. Sometimes we can remember events in vivid detail, while other times we may only have a vague sense of familiarity.

The first type, a recollected memory, is one that is remembered with strong details about the time at which it was learned—such as a dining experience the previous evening, where not only was the lobster dinner recalled, but also were the paintings on the wall and the restaurant staff who served you.

On the other hand, a familiar memory is similar to a recollected one in that it is known, but differs in that it is recalled without any explicit details surrounding the event. That is, a familiar memory lacks specifics about the setting, like the waiter who served dinner or what the décor was.

This video demonstrates how to combine functional magnetic resonance imaging—fMRI—with a task called Remember-Know to investigate how the brain—especially the hippocampus—responds to judgments made towards repeated or novel images based on previous work performed by Gimbel and Brewer.

In this experiment, participants are asked to complete two phases: initial encoding and fMRI testing. In part one, encoding, they are exposed to colored pictures of nameable objects, such as an apple, which they must remember.

Following each item’s presentation, a question is asked, promoting participants’ attention during this process.

Afterwards, in the second phase—fMRI testing—participants are placed inside a scanner and, via a projection system, are shown images: those previously observed along with brand new ones.

A fixation cross precedes each picture to optimize the separation of the brain’s hemodynamic responses across the different presentations.

Upon seeing each image, participants are asked to respond in one of three ways: ‘remember’ if the item can be recalled along with specific details about its presentation; ‘know’, if it’s familiar but they cannot recall specific details about seeing it before; or ‘new’, if the object was not seen at all.

In this case, the dependent variable is the intensity of the hemodynamic signal measured after each response type. The extent of activation can then be visualized into clusters of voxels on an anatomical brain scan.

The hippocampus—a region in the medial temporal lobe notably studied in learning and memory studies—is expected to show greater activation during the ‘remember’ trials than during the ‘know’ and ‘new’ trials.

These findings would support a dual-process theory of memory recall, where the hippocampus supports recollection and a different neural region—one outside of the hippocampus—generates familiarity.

For experimental control and safety concerns, recruit participants who are right-handed, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no history of psychological disorders or suffering from claustrophobia, and without any metal in their body.

Have them fill out a magnetic resonance screening form, with additional questions related to their health and safety encompassing the scanning session.

Before sending the participant into the scanner, sit in front of a laptop and expose them to objects that they need to remember for the next session. Explain that they will now view 256 color images, each for 3 s. To ensure that they are paying attention, instruct them to press the ‘F’ key to indicate that an object is living or ‘J’ if the item is non-living.

After the participant views all of the images, further explain that those pictures, along with an additional 256 novel items, will be shown inside the scanner. Also introduce them to the MR-safe button-box that they will use to classify items—as ‘remember’, ‘know’, or ‘novel’—when they appear onscreen.

In preparation to enter the scanning room, ask the participant to remove all metal objects from their body, including cell phones, watches or jewelry, wallets, keys, belts, and coins, due to the strong magnetic field. Use a metal detector to verify that no metal items remain.

Next, escort the participant near the scanner. Provide earplugs to protect their ears from loud noises and earphones so that they can hear you during the scan. Have them lie down on the bed with their head in the coil, and secure it with foam pads to avoid excessive movement and blurring during the scan.

Place a mirror above the participant’s eyes to reflect a screen at the back of the scanner bore. Make sure that they are equipped with a squeeze ball in case of an emergency during the scan and the button response box. Also, remind them that it’s very important to keep their head as still as possible throughout the experiment.

After raising the scanner bed, align the participant and send them into the bore. In the adjacent room, collect high-resolution anatomical images before starting the event-related, functional phase. Synchronize the start of the stimulus presentation with the start of the functional scan, and allow the participant to complete 512 trials.

To conclude the session, bring them out of the scanning room. Debrief them by providing an explanation of the study and compensation for their participation.

To begin the analysis, first pre-process the data by performing correction to reduce motion artifacts, temporal filtering to remove signal drifts, and spatial smoothing to increase the signal-to-noise ratio.

Then, create a model of the expected hemodynamic response for each task condition. Fit the data to this model, resulting in a statistical map for each subject, where the value at each voxel represents the extent to which that voxel was involved in the task condition.

Register the participant’s brain to a standard atlas to combine data across subjects. To perform a group-level analysis, threshold the statistical maps, taking into account correction for multiple comparisons. Only accept significant voxels if they also occur within a cluster of a given size to minimize false-positive results.

Using these extracted clusters, overlay them on an average anatomical brain. Note that the activation measured during the ‘know’ trials was subtracted from that in the ‘remember’ trials. The hippocampus, outlined here in yellow, showed significantly more activation for ‘remember’ trials compared to ‘know’ trials.

To examine hippocampal activation in more detail, plot the percentage of signal change across time after the onset of the stimulus.

Inspection of this time-course of activity revealed that the hippocampus responded positively when participants explicitly reported remembering the stimuli and when identifying new stimuli—noted here with a positive deflection.

In contrast, it responded negatively or very little when participants reported feelings of familiarity or did not remember images at all.

These results support a dual process theory of memory recall, where the hippocampus is involved with memory recollection but not familiarity.

Now that you are familiar with designing an fMRI experiment to understand brain activation during judgments of recollection and familiarity in typical adults, let’s look at additional studies that apply the Remember-Know paradigm.

If the hippocampus plays a central role in recollection, its absence might reveal dissociations in memory retrieval. This scenario can be addressed by comparing patients with bilateral hippocampal damage versus controls—individuals without any such damage.

Interestingly, patients with damage showed impaired memory recollection compared to controls, whereas both groups performed equally well during familiarity trials. Taken together, these results support a specific role of the hippocampus in recollection processes.

On the contrary, if individuals showed increased hippocampal volumes, we’d predict that they’d also display enhanced recollection.

One such example exists and involves London taxicab drivers, who were shown to augment their hippocampal gray matter after years of memorizing extensive and complex routes around the city. With their larger hippocampi and superb memory, they transport passengers to their correct destination in a timely manner.

Researchers are also interested in gaining further insight into the mechanisms responsible for memory retrieval in order to enhance it in other ways. Take for instance, a college psychology lecture, where large amounts of information are presented. Knowing that material is familiar is not helpful for an exam.

Instead, a student needs something else—beyond having that cup of coffee—to aid in remembering. Perhaps, taking a memory-enhancing compound would allow improved recall of the entire discussion to ace that important test.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s introduction to Remember-Know task. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design and conduct the memory recall experiment in conjunction with functional neuroimaging, how to analyze and interpret differential brain activation results, and finally how to apply the paradigm to real-life scenarios.

Thanks for watching!

Tags

Cite This
JoVE Science Education Database. JoVE Science Education. Learning and Memory: The Remember-Know Task. JoVE, Cambridge, MA, (2023).