21,949 Views
•
05:52 min
•
May 17, 2011
DOI:
The overall goal of this procedure is to observe the anxiety experience. When mice are confronted with a novel food in a novel place, first, select a highly palatable food. Next, select the novel environment in which the animal will experience the food.
Once prepared, introduce the animal and record behavioral parameters. Ultimately, results can be obtained that show changes in anxiety level through changes in eating latency. Reading the hyperphagia literature, I found that people often used very large arenas in which to place the food to provoke anxiety.
But you can provoke anxiety with a very much smaller test arena. The result is that you don’t get your hyperphagia latency data contaminated by a lot of exploratory activity. Also, if the test compound or the lesion that you’re testing diminishes motor activity, then that would be also a confound, which would be much more pronounced with a large arena.
But the small test arena will ensure that the mouse, even if it is very slow to move, does find the food fairly quickly. The whole idea of these tests is so that the mouse goes into the test apparatus and finds the food virtually instantly, and then you get a pure hyperphagia latency data The evening before testing, remove all food from the home cage except for one gram of chow per mouse. Break the pieces apart to be sure that group housed animals eat equal shares.
Next, choose the type of hyperphagia test apparatus to be used. Four types were developed with slight differences. It is important that dry bait be distributed evenly over the apparatus’s floor to reduce the need for exploration.
In this example, liquid bait is contained within a food well. Baits of extreme palatability encourage the tasting of novel food and only require mild food deprivation seen. Here are examples of palatable foods that can be used as bait to set up the test apparatus.
Glue a lead weight onto the base of a translucent plastic jug about 15 centimeters in diameter with a spout protruding of further two centimeters, overturn the jug onto a white plastic base. So the S spout covers a food well. Since odor may play a significant role in this test, allow non-experimental mice to explore the apparatus before testing starts.
Next, align a row of small cages. Place one mouse into each cage about five minutes before testing and return them to the same cage. After each trial, fill the food well with full cream sweetened condensed milk, diluted 50 50 with water to serve as the bait.
To begin the test, place a mouse onto the platform facing away from the well. Lower the jug gently into position. Taking care not to trap the tail.
Measure the latency to eat. This is defined as eating or drinking continuously. For two to three seconds, count any fecal boli and note any urination.
Remove the animal after two minutes. If a mouse is not drunk, by then return it to its cage and retest it around three minutes later. Most animals drink within three trials before the next trial.
Clean the food well and the plastic base with a moist cloth, followed by a dry cloth and refill the food well. Hippocampal lesions produce a strong decrease in hyperphagia in mice. The latency figures in hyperphagia testing are typically highly variable.
The histogram shows data from three successive hyperphagia tests in a single cohort of mice. Following this procedure, the animals can be tested in different anxiety tests such as the light dark box or the plus maze. However, we now know that anxiety as is manifest in a number of different forms, and food related anxiety may not necessarily be the same as anxiety as measured in the light dark box or the plus maze, for example.
So we should always test any treatment that we’re administering to the mice in all three anxiety tests or as many as we can in order to ensure that when the compound goes to the clinic, it’s more likely to act as a anxiolytic in humans.
Mice and rats, due to their innate cautiousness, are initially slow in consuming a novel food, particularly in a novel place. This hyponeophagia can readily be measured in the laboratory, even though laboratory animals are much less anxious than their wild counterparts
05:05
The Successive Alleys Test of Anxiety in Mice and Rats
Related Videos
21696 Views
07:12
Use of the Open Field Maze to Measure Locomotor and Anxiety-like Behavior in Mice
Related Videos
73012 Views
06:50
Systematic Assessment of Well-Being in Mice for Procedures Using General Anesthesia
Related Videos
12163 Views
09:09
Elevated Plus Maze for Mice
Related Videos
90615 Views
08:26
Assessing Activity-based Anorexia in Mice
Related Videos
12454 Views
07:13
The Unpredictable Chronic Mild Stress Protocol for Inducing Anhedonia in Mice
Related Videos
17387 Views
14:40
Optogenetic Manipulation of Neuronal Activity to Modulate Behavior in Freely Moving Mice
Related Videos
18107 Views
05:23
Investigating Migraine-Like Behavior Using Light Aversion in Mice
Related Videos
3731 Views
05:15
Intermittent Binge-Intake Model in Mice
Related Videos
764 Views
04:20
Social Isolation Model: A Noninvasive Rodent Model of Stress and Anxiety
Related Videos
4197 Views
Read Article
Cite this Article
Deacon, R. M. Hyponeophagia: A Measure of Anxiety in the Mouse. J. Vis. Exp. (51), e2613, doi:10.3791/2613 (2011).
Copy